TY - JOUR
T1 - Observer bias in randomized clinical trials with time-to-event outcomes
T2 - systematic review of trials with both blinded and non-blinded outcome assessors
AU - Hróbjartsson, Asbjørn
AU - Thomsen, Ann Sofia Skou
AU - Emanuelsson, Frida
AU - Tendal, Britta
AU - Rasmussen, Jeppe Vejlgaard
AU - Hilden, Jørgen
AU - Boutron, Isabelle
AU - Ravaud, Philippe
AU - Brorson, Stig
N1 - © The Author 2014; all rights reserved. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the International Epidemiological Association.
PY - 2014/6
Y1 - 2014/6
N2 - Background: We wanted to evaluate the impact of nonblinded outcome assessors on estimated treatment effects in time-to-event trials. Methods: Systematic review of randomized clinical trials with both blinded and nonblinded assessors of the same time-to-event outcome. Two authors agreed on inclusion of trials and outcomes. We compared hazard ratios based on nonblinded and blinded assessments. A ratio of hazard ratios (RHR) <1 indicated that nonblinded assessors generated more optimistic effect estimates. We pooled RHRs with inverse variance random-effects meta-analysis. Results: We included 18 trials. Eleven trials (1969 patients) with subjective outcomes provided hazard ratios, RHR 0.88 (0.69 to 1.12), (I2 = 44%, P = 0.06), but unconditional pooling was problematic because of qualitative heterogeneity. Four atypical cytomegalovirus retinitis trials compared experimental oral administration with control intravenous administration of the same drug, resulting in bias favouring the control intervention, RHR 1.33 (0.98 to 1.82). Seven trials of cytomegalovirus retinitis, tibial fracture and multiple sclerosis compared experimental interventions with standard control interventions, e.g. placebo, no-treatment or active control, resulting in bias favouring the experimental intervention, RHR 0.73 (0.57 to 0.93), indicating an average exaggeration of nonblinded hazard ratios by 27% (7% to 43%). Conclusions: Lack of blinded outcome assessors in randomized trials with subjective time-to-event outcomes causes high risk of observer bias. Nonblinded outcome assessors typically favour the experimental intervention, exaggerating the hazard ratio by an average of approximately 27%; but in special situations, nonblinded outcome assessors favour control interventions, inducing a comparable degree of observer bias in the reversed direction.
AB - Background: We wanted to evaluate the impact of nonblinded outcome assessors on estimated treatment effects in time-to-event trials. Methods: Systematic review of randomized clinical trials with both blinded and nonblinded assessors of the same time-to-event outcome. Two authors agreed on inclusion of trials and outcomes. We compared hazard ratios based on nonblinded and blinded assessments. A ratio of hazard ratios (RHR) <1 indicated that nonblinded assessors generated more optimistic effect estimates. We pooled RHRs with inverse variance random-effects meta-analysis. Results: We included 18 trials. Eleven trials (1969 patients) with subjective outcomes provided hazard ratios, RHR 0.88 (0.69 to 1.12), (I2 = 44%, P = 0.06), but unconditional pooling was problematic because of qualitative heterogeneity. Four atypical cytomegalovirus retinitis trials compared experimental oral administration with control intravenous administration of the same drug, resulting in bias favouring the control intervention, RHR 1.33 (0.98 to 1.82). Seven trials of cytomegalovirus retinitis, tibial fracture and multiple sclerosis compared experimental interventions with standard control interventions, e.g. placebo, no-treatment or active control, resulting in bias favouring the experimental intervention, RHR 0.73 (0.57 to 0.93), indicating an average exaggeration of nonblinded hazard ratios by 27% (7% to 43%). Conclusions: Lack of blinded outcome assessors in randomized trials with subjective time-to-event outcomes causes high risk of observer bias. Nonblinded outcome assessors typically favour the experimental intervention, exaggerating the hazard ratio by an average of approximately 27%; but in special situations, nonblinded outcome assessors favour control interventions, inducing a comparable degree of observer bias in the reversed direction.
KW - Humans
KW - Observer Variation
KW - Randomized Controlled Trials as Topic
KW - Research Design
KW - Single-Blind Method
U2 - 10.1093/ije/dyt270
DO - 10.1093/ije/dyt270
M3 - Journal article
C2 - 24448109
SN - 0300-5771
VL - 43
SP - 937
EP - 948
JO - International Journal of Epidemiology
JF - International Journal of Epidemiology
IS - 3
ER -